Skip to main content
SearchLoginLogin or Signup

Social Distance and Immediate Informal Responses to Violent Victimization

There are a number of ways that victims of violence informally handle attacks as they unfold. Their responses range in severity from physical resistance, to talking it out with the offender, to running away, to cooperating. Why do victims respond in a more or less severe ...

Published onSep 07, 2012
Social Distance and Immediate Informal Responses to Violent Victimization
·
history

You're viewing an older Release (#3) of this Pub.

  • This Release (#3) was created on Jun 19, 2020 ()
  • The latest Release (#4) was created on Jan 30, 2023 ().

Note: This is the postprint of the following paper; publisher version available here.

Jacques, Scott, and Callie Rennison. 2013. Social Distance and Immediate Informal Responses to Violent Victimization. Journal of Interpersonal Violence 28:733-753.

Abstract: There are a number of ways that victims of violence informally handle attacks as they unfold. Their responses range in severity from physical resistance, to talking it out with the offender, to running away, to cooperating. Why do victims respond in a more or less severe manner? Cooney (2009) suggests that social distance is part of the answer: the further the relational or cultural distance between offender and victim, the more severe the latter's response. Using National Crime Victimization Survey data, we test hypotheses derived from this theory and find oppositional findings. Specifically, results indicate that closer social distance predicts more severe responses. We conclude by discussing the implications of this finding for future work, especially as relates to the study of self-protective behavior.

Introduction

In the course of being attacked, why does a victim react in a more or less severe manner? Why, for example, does one victim fight back but another argues with the offender, runs away, or cooperates? Building on previous work nested in the paradigm of pure sociology (Black, 1976, 1998), Cooney (2009) proposed a new answer to this question: the greater the social distance between victim and offender, the more severe the reaction by the victimized person. In this paper, we test hypotheses derived from this theory using National Crime Victimization Survey (NCVS) data. To our knowledge, this paper provides the first quantitative examination of its kind. We begin by providing a brief overview of social control and Cooney’s (2009) purely sociological theory of it. Then we specify our hypotheses and outline the data, sample, measures and analytic approach. Finally, we present and discuss our results, including implications for future research and theory development.

Theorizing the Severity of Informal Social Control

Social control is a response to wrongdoing  (Black, 1976, 1998, 2011). It may be formal or informal (Black, 1976, 1983), and reflexive or delayed (Jacobs and Wright, 2006). Formal social control refers to governmental responses to wrongdoing, such as arresting, prosecuting, or imprisoning an offender. Responses to deviance not involving the government are considered informal social control. Both formal and informal responses may be enacted reflexively, meaning during the initial transgression, or delayed.

Reflexive informal control includes a plethora of behaviors: self-defense, struggling, ducking, blocking blows, holding onto one’s property, chasing after or holding onto the offender; arguing, reasoning, pleading, or bargaining with the offender; running or driving away and hiding; cooperating or doing nothing. These responses are typologized in many ways. One approach is to divide them into forms of control (Black, 1998) based on who takes action in the course of managing conflict – the victim, the offender, both, neither, or a third-party – and which action is done by each party. Four forms of reflexive information control are considered in this manuscript: self-help, negotiation, avoidance, and toleration.[1] Self-help is a unilateral response by the victim against the transgressor. Negotiation refers to when disputants “talk out” the problem. Avoidance indicates a victim curtailing interaction with the wrongdoer. Toleration occurs when a victim takes no action beyond cooperation with the offender.

Each form of control has received ample academic attention in a variety of disciplines (for a modern-day sociological example, see Anderson, 1999; for historical and anthropological examples, see Black, 1998). Prior work tends to look at these forms of control as distinct behaviors rather than conceptualize and theorize them as quantitatively distinct levels of a single variable. The questions for such studies are: Why do victims react to offenders with self-help? Or negotiation? Or avoidance? Or toleration? In other words, typically the emphasis has been on explaining individual forms of control one at a time, with different propositions for each. This approach is less than ideal as it reduces a theory’s parsimony (see Popper, 2002 [1959]). An exception to the piecemeal approach is research that groups the forms of control into the broad categories of “forceful” and “nonforceful” (see, e.g., Bachman et al., 2002; Block & Skogan, 1986). This distinction is simple, but maybe too simple as prior research indicates that different kinds of forceful and nonforceful responses have diverse effects on victimization (see Tark & Kleck, 2002: 867). A good typology is simple (Cooney and Phillips, 2002) but should not come at the cost of obscuring important differences (Tark & Kleck, 2002).

Cooney (2009) suggested an intriguing way of conceptualizing the distinct forms of informal control as a single variable with multiple levels. He argues that each form rests at a different spot on a continuum of severity. “The more severe the sanction, the more people and groups shy away from it” (p. 204). Thus the forms of control can be “arranged in order of how strongly they repel social actors” (p. 10): self-help being the most severe, followed by negotiation (especially that involving compensation), avoidance, and toleration. If valid, this continuum is an important idea because it brings these forms of social control together and thereby facilitates parsimonious explanations. Cooney’s conceptualization raises the question: Why do victims react to offenders with more or less severe informal control?

The Perspective of Pure Sociology

One answer to that question is nested in the paradigm of pure sociology (Black, 1976, 1995, 1998).[2] Although there are many sociological theories of control, what distinguishes purely sociological theory is the unit of analysis (Black, 1995). Typically sociological theories examine groups (see, e.g., Messner and Rosenfeld, 2007) or individuals (e.g., Hirschi, 1969). Yet any given group or individual does not always behave the same way across all of their interactions. For example, even the most violent individuals do not commit retaliation in all of their conflicts (Cooney, 1998, 2009; Phillips, 2003). This suggests there is more to explaining social control than simply analyzing differences between individuals or groups.[3]

Pure sociology’s unit of analysis is the social geometry of the case. A case is any given social interaction, such as a violent victimization or act of social control. Each case has a “social geometry” (Black, 1995), which is the social status of and social distance between persons involved in the interaction. For example, violent victimizations include an offender and a victim. Some offenders and some victims have more wealth, employment, family life, education, or respectability than others.  The more a person has these things, the higher that person’s status. In addition, some offenders and victims are more familiar or culturally alike than others. The better two persons know each other or the more they share beliefs and modes of expression (e.g., language), the closer is the social distance between them. Pure sociologists explore how variation in the social status of and social distance between persons involved in an interaction – the “social geometry” of the case – influences their actions.

Cooney’s Theory of Informal Control Severity

Cooney (2009) proposed a “purely sociological” theory of why people respond toward offenders with a more or less severe form of control. His goal was to delineate how the handling of violent incidents, specifically homicides, is affected by the social status of and social distance between killers, murder victims, and other persons who could conceivably become involved in the case (e.g., family members). With an eye toward developing a general theory, his explanation is grounded on findings from modern, historical and anthropological studies that involve a collection of diverse methods: “detailed firsthand observations, experimental manipulations, statistical analyses, historical narratives, in-depth interviews, and the like” (p. 33-5).

This diversity of information led to a simple theory. Cooney (2009) provides five propositions: Informal control is more severe when (1) a low status person offends against a high status person rather than vice versa; (2) the victim becomes higher in status; (3) the offender becomes lower in status; (4) the status of the victim and offender increases; and, (5) the offense is among less intimate or culturally similar persons.[4]

Examples drawn from Cooney’s (2009) secondary research illustrates these propositions. The effect of social status is evident in how “[a]mong groups as diverse as medieval Icelanders…, medieval and early modern Irish…, Tibetan pastoralists…, the Ifugao of the Phillipines and the Mae Enga of New Guinea…, the wealthier the victim, the more compensation the killer must pay” (p. 58). And “the shooting of a leading gang member—a ‘ghetto star’—elevates not only the probability but the scale of vengeance: the survivors are more likely to kill and kill several times than they would be for the homicide of a rank-and-file member” (p. 85).

The effect of social distance becomes clear when considering that “[i]n the postbellum South, offenses (such as homicide) by whites against whites led to a lynching much less often than offenses by blacks against white. Moreover, when lynched, whites were less often tortured or mutilated than blacks” (p. 151). Similarly, “among the Mataco of the Gran Chaco,… located in central South America, the tendency to seek vengeance or compensation for a homicide is abrogated when the victim is a family member” (p. 179).[5]

The Present Study

The goal of this research is to quantitatively test Cooney’s theory of informal control severity as pertains to victims’ reactions toward offenders in the course of a violent victimization. Specifically, the purpose of this research is to better understand why victims reflexively respond to attacks with self-help, negotiation, avoidance, or toleration. We test Cooney’s (2009) proposition that the immorality of offending is a direct function of social distance (i.e., relational and cultural distance). We hypothesize that victims use less severe forms of informal control during a victimization when they are (1) familiar with victimizers rather than strangers and/or (2) of the same race as them rather than different. For methodological reasons, we restrain our examination to the effect of social distance on reflexive informal control. The NCVS cannot be used to fully test the influence of social status on reflexive informal control because this survey does not systematically gather information on offenders’ income, employment, marital status, or formal education. Though this information is gathered for the victim, the lack of data for offenders entails the inability to examine the difference in status between an offender and victim.

Data

We test variants of our hypothesis using 1993 to 2009 NCVS data. These data are gathered continuously, in-person and over the phone from a nationally representative household sample. On average each year approximately 160,000 persons in 80,000 households are interviewed. Historically, response rates associated with the NCVS have been high (in the 90 and greater percentiles for households, and from the mid 80 percentiles and greater for individuals); for details on the NCVS methodology see Rennison and Rand (2007). The data are gathered using a series of screener questions identifying if a respondent was victimized during the preceding six months. Additional survey questions collected on an incident form gathers detailed incident characteristics regarding the victim, offender, and incident.

The NCVS offers several advantages for this research. First, it is characterized by a large sample of victimizations over a 17 year period. Second, the data are representative of non-institutionalized victims of crime age 12 or older. Third, the NCVS offers detailed incident-level information including the relationship between the victim and offender, weapon presence and the form of reflexive response enacted by victims during the incident. Finally, NCVS data contain information on victimizations regardless of whether the police were notified.

Sample

Our sample consists of attempted and completed non-fatal violent victimizations in which the victim came face to face with the offender and used reflexive informal control (e.g., attacked offender with a weapon, hid, cooperated, etc.). These victimizations include attempted and completed rape, sexual assault, robbery, aggravated assault and simple assault. Rape is defined as forced sexual intercourse that includes psychological coercion and physical force, including heterosexual and homosexual rape, committed against both males and females. Attempted rape includes verbal threats of rape. Sexual assault consists of incidents involving attacks or attempted attacks generally associated with unwanted sexual contact between victim and offender. Sexual assaults may or may not involve force and include such behavior as grabbing or fondling; they also include verbal threats. Robbery constitutes property or cash taken directly from a person by use or threat of force, with or without a weapon, and with or without injury. Aggravated assault is defined as an actual or attempted attack with a weapon, regardless of whether injury resulted, or an attack or attempted attack without a weapon when serious injury results. Finally, simple assault involves an attack without a weapon resulting in either minor injury such as a bruise, cut, scrape or scratch or no injury.

Further, our analyses are limited to violence involving white and black victims and offenders (i.e., white-on-white, white-on-black, black-on-white, and black-on-black violence) as the research requires we identify when victim and offender race is similar or different. While NCVS data provides multiple categories for victim’s race (e.g., white, black, Asian, American Indian, multiple races) it only provides four offender racial categories (white, black, other and unknown). It is unclear if “other” offenders correspond to Asian, American Indian, and mixed race victims, thus only white and black victims and offenders were retained in the working file. These criteria resulted in a working file of 20,603 violent victimizations (unweighted) occurring from 1993 to 2009.

Measures and Analytic Approach

We model the degree to which different forms of reflexive informal control are used by victims as a function of social distance between the victim and offender. The dependent variable is reflexive informal control engaged in by the victim during the victimization. It is measured using four categories ranked from most to least severe: self-help, negotiation, avoidance, and toleration. Self-help refers to instances in which the victim threatened or successfully attacked the offender physically or with a weapon; it also includes instances when the victim chased off the offender and defended themselves or their property. Negotiation indicates that the victim argued or reasoned with the offender. Avoidance entails when the victim ran away, hid or locked the door to protect them self. Toleration indicates the victim did nothing or cooperated with the offender. Victimizations in which the victim engaged in multiple forms of reflexive informal control were coded at the most severe level.

The independent variable, social distance, is measured two ways. In some models it is measured as a combination of its two components: relational distance and cultural distance. Relational distance describes the closeness of the victim and offender: 0=known and 1=stranger.[6] Cultural distance identifies the similarity/difference between victim and offender race. Intraracial violence is considered closer in cultural distance while inter-racial violence is considered further (0=intraracial and 1=inter-racial). When social distance is presented as a combination of cultural and relational distance, four categories are used: 0=intraracial, known participants, 1=intraracial, strangers, 2= inter-racial, known participants, and 3=inter-racial violence between strangers. In other models, the two components of social distance are analyzed separately to identify the contribution of each.

Additional relevant correlates of informal reflexive control are controlled for in the models. These include measures of the victim’s social status (wealth, employment, marital status, level of formal education), the type of violent victimization, victim and offender gender and age, and weapon presence.[7]

To test the hypotheses, binary logistic regression is utilized.[8] Analyses accounted for the complex sample design of the NCVS using Stata’s survey weighting regression procedures (Hosmer & Lemeshow, 2000).

Results

An examination of the working file indicates that during violent victimization, victims were most likely to practice toleration during the attack (41%), followed by self-help (36%), avoidance (14%), and lastly negotiation (9%). Most of the victimizations in the sample involved victims and offenders of the same race who knew one another (51%). Thirty-one percent of the violence was characterized by greater social distance (same race, strangers). Six percent of the violence involved known parties of different races.  Finally, 12% of the violence occurred between victims and offenders of different races who were strangers. Most incidents were simple assaults (66%) and involve victims who were never married (57%), are male (56%), and residing in a suburban area (47%).

- - TABLE I ABOUT HERE - -

Social Distance & Reflexive Informal Control

The first series of binary logistic regression models presented in Table II investigate the influence of social distance on the severity of victims’ reflexive informal control. These models operationalize social distance (the independent variable) using four-categories representing the combination of relational and cultural distance. Three hypotheses are considered:

  1. Reflexive self-help is more likely than negotiation, avoidance, or toleration among offenders and victims who are strangers and/or of different races rather than known to each and/or of same race.

  2. Reflexive self-help or negotiation is more likely than avoidance or toleration among offenders and victims who are strangers and/or of different races rather than known to each and/or of same race.

  3. Reflexive self-help, negotiation, or avoidance is more likely than toleration among offenders and victims who are strangers and/or of different races rather than known to each and/or of same race.

Panel A in Table II examines the influence of social distance on forms of reflexive informal control measured as self-help (e.g., attacking with a weapon; coded as 1) versus less severe behaviors such as screaming, hiding, running away or reasoning with the offender (i.e., negotiation, avoidance and toleration; coded as 0). Panel B compares self-help and negotiation (coded as 1) and avoidance and toleration (coded as 0). Panel C compares self-help, negotiation and avoidance (coded as 1) to the least severe form of informal control, toleration (coded as 0).

 - - TABLE II ABOUT HERE - -

Results in all three panels fail to offer support for the social distance—reflexive informal control hypotheses. Specifically, findings show that the greater the social distance between the victim and offender, the less likely the victim will use the most severe forms of informal control. Panel A indicates that reflexive self-help is significantly less likely than negotiation, avoidance, or toleration when offenders and victims have greater social distance. In terms of odds ratios, results show that moving from a known, similar race victim/offender to a stranger, similar race victim/offender reduces the odds of self-help (compared to negotiation, avoidance or toleration) 21%.  Increasing social distance from known similar race victim/offender to known different race victim/offender decreases the odds of self-help 24%. And results show that moving from a known, similar race victim/offender to a stranger different race victim/offender victimization reduces the odds of reflexive self-help (compared to negotiation, avoidance or toleration) 34%. 

Panel B offers similarly unexpected findings. Reflexive self-help or negotiation is less likely than avoidance or toleration as social distance grows. Specifically, reflexive self-help or negotiation is significantly less likely than avoidance or toleration as social distance increases. Findings indicate that moving from a known similar race victim/offender victimization to a stranger similar race victim/offender reduces the odds of self-help and negotiation (compared to avoidance or toleration) 21%.  In addition, increasing social distance from known similar race victim/offender to known different race victim/offender decreases the odds of self-help 16%. And finally, the analysis shows that moving from a known, similar race victim/offender to a stranger of a different race victim/offender reduces the odds of reflexive self-help or negotiation (compared to avoidance or toleration) 37%. 

Finding in Panel C are similar to those in Panels A and B. Reflexive self-help, negotiation, or avoidance is less likely than toleration as social distance increases. Reflexive self-help, negotiation or avoidance is significantly less likely than toleration as social distance increases. Adjusted odds ratios show that moving from a known, similar race victim/offender to a stranger similar race victim/offender reduces the odds of self-help/negotiation or avoidance (compared to toleration) 11%. Moving from known similar race victim/offender to known different race victim/offender decreases the odds of self-help 12%. And results indicate that moving from a known, similar race victim/offender to a stranger of a different race victim/offender reduces the odds of reflexive self-help/negotiation or avoidance (compared to toleration) 24%. 

Social Distance Disaggregated

An alternative approach to testing the hypothesis is to disaggregate social distance into its two components: cultural distance and relational distance. Presented in combination (as is done in Table II), each component of social distance’s independent contribution is obscured. Thus, the following three models take the same form as those in the previous section with the exception that relational distance and cultural distance are entered separately. By disaggregating social distance in the way, one is able to examine the relative contribution of each component of social distance in influencing reflexive informal control behaviors.

- - TABLE III ABOUT HERE - -

Results in the three Panels in Table III do not offer support for the social distance—reflexive informal control hypotheses. In fact, findings in each model indicate that the greater the social distance between the victim and offender, the less likely the victim will use the most severe forms of informal SP. Panel A results show that reflexive self-help is significantly less likely than negotiation, avoidance, or toleration as cultural distance increases. In other words, moving from a similar race to a different race dyad, reduces the odds of reflexive self-help (compared to negotiation, avoidance or toleration) 19%.  Similar findings obtain when considering relational distance. As relational distance increases, the odds of reflexive self-help (compared to negotiation, avoidance or toleration) decreases 21%.

Panel B offers no support for the hypotheses. As cultural and relational distance increase, the odds of reflexive self-help or negotiation (versus avoidance or toleration) decrease. Specifically, as cultural distance increases, the odds of reflexive self-help or negotiation (compared to avoidance or toleration) decreases 19%.  And as relational distance increases, the odds of   reflexive self-help or negotiation (compared to avoidance or toleration) decreases 23%.  

Finally, Panel C reveals that reflexive self-help, negotiation, or avoidance is less likely than toleration as cultural and relational distance increases. Adjusted odds ratios show that an increase in cultural distance (i.e., moving from same race to different race victim/offender dyad)   reduces the odds of reflexive self-help, negotiation or avoidance (compared to toleration) 13%.  And an increase in relational distance (from known to stranger victim/offender dyad) reduces the odds of reflexive self-help, negotiation or avoidance (compared to toleration) 13%.  

Discussion & Conclusion

During an attack, a victim can informally respond in a number of ways: tolerate; avoid; negotiate; retaliate (Black, 1998). With data from the NCVS and while controlling for relevant correlates, we tested variants of the hypothesis that when victims and offenders are known to each other rather than strangers, or of the same race instead of different ones, they are more likely to utilize less severe forms of informal control (Cooney, 2009). The findings indicate a significant role of social distance in explaining the forms of informal control—but not in the predicted direction. As social distance between victims and offenders increased, the chance of more severe forms of informal control decreased: self-help was more likely than the less severe forms; self-help and negotiation less likely than avoidance and toleration; toleration more likely than the more severe forms. Our findings do not support Cooney’s (2009) theory.

Our research has limitations that may have affected the results and, if possible, should be circumvented down the road. For one, the NCVS does not include victimization of persons under age 12, nor does it offer insight into violence against institutionalized persons (e.g., prisoners) or the homeless. It is possible that the NCVS disproportionately undercounts victimization between intimates (Straus, 1999), which would obviously distort our findings. Finally, it is important to note that the reflexive behavior measures were based on victim responses; it could be that respondents misstate or misremember their behavior.

Regarding limitations in testing Cooney’s (2009) theory, we focused on reactions to a specific kind of conflict – violent victimization – but Cooney’s theory is meant to explain reactions to all kinds of conflicts, from theft to vandalism to adultery and beyond. Another limitation of our analysis is that it focuses on violent crime victims only while Cooney’s theory explains reactions of partisans (e.g., friends, family, and police) as well as victims. Relatedly, Cooney’s propositions also explain settlement, which is when a neutral third-party handles the offense, and praise; inclusion of these forms may change the results. Our measures are not as precise as preferred; relational distance is measured as a discrete rather than continuous variable, and the measure of cultural distance is limited and indirect. And although we included some aspects of social status in our models, we were not able to fully analyze and control for their effects; thus this portion of Cooney’s theory remains untested.  

Conflict as the Unit of Analysis & Self-Protective Behavior

In some respects, another limitation of our paper is it focuses on immediate responses but Cooney’s (2009) theory also explains delayed ones. As described above, the unit of analysis for this explanation of social control is the social geometry of conflict. Our unit of analysis has been the victimization situation, which is not exactly the same as conflict. A situation is a self-contained interaction, but “[a] conflict can begin and end in the same situation if the grievance and response occur in the same interaction” or “can involve multiple situations if the disputants continue to engage the grievance across multiple interactions, perhaps even for years on end” (Phillips, 2003: 674). At the extreme, a victim may delay vengeance for decades (for examples, see Cooney, 1998). Thus, the optimum test of Cooney’s (2009) theory would involve following victims for years after the initial affront – up to the point they are dead – to see whether the offense is controlled. While this is possible to accomplish, it is obviously difficult in practice. Therefore tests of his theory will out of practical necessity need to restrict themselves to a more restricted timeframe – such as the victimization situation.

On that note, Cooney’s theory and the findings of this paper have implications for another body of research. Studies in “self-protective behavior”, or “SP” for short, are concerned with how and why people respond when being victimized, and the effect of reactions on offending outcomes (see, e.g., Block & Skogan, 1986; Kleck & DeLone, 1993; Melde & Rennison, 2010; Tark & Kleck, 2004).Thus, reflexive control and SP are different terms for the same behavior. This means the results of our analysis – greater social distance leads to less severe SP – deserve future attention from scholars working in this area. Why is it that greater social distance leads to less severe SP actions? Is it simply that greater social distance results in less severe informal SP, despite Cooney (2009) theorizing otherwise? Or are there other perspectives/theories that suggest a mediating factor? Or is it that the continuum of severity proposed by Cooney (2009) is conceptually off the mark?

Our results provide empirical grounding for a theory of SP severity. Although they could be interpreted through multiple frameworks, the predominant one in research on SP is the rationality perspective (see, e.g., Tark & Kleck, 2004). This framework argues behavior is guided by a risk-reward calculation that weighs the benefits and costs of alternative lines of action. The one with the greatest overall utility should be the one enacted (Clarke & Cornish, 1985). When seen through this theoretical lens, SP is concerned with minimizing personal injury or loss of possessions and obtaining retributive benefits by punishing the offender through formal or informal control (e.g., arrest by police). Social distance and SP severity may vary inversely because reputational concerns are at greater stake when being attacked by a stranger than acquaintance, friend, or family member, which results in a less tolerant response. Moreover, in interracial or stranger-on-stranger offenses the victim may feel less able to predict the attacker’s actions (and counter-actions) and, therefore, take a more peaceable approach. Thus, while victims may perceive offenses by strangers or racially different persons to be more immoral and deserving of more severe responses, it may be irrational to act on such perceptions during the course of being victimized. These hypotheses can be tested by analyzing whether (1) victims attribute different levels of utility to the various forms of control (2) depending on their social distance from offenders, and (3) if this affects which form of control is used by victims toward offenders during victimizations.

Rather than theorize Cooney’s (2009) conceptualization of SP severity, another approach is to question its validity and, if necessary, refurbish it. The value of this conceptualization is it brings the forms of social control together by turning them into a ordered quantitative variable, which allows for more parsimonious theory. But if the conceptualization is invalid then explanations of it will be inherently limited. While it seems reasonable to rank toleration – the absence of control – as the least severe of all, it is unclear why Cooney ranks self-help as more severe than negotiation, or it as more severe than avoidance (see Moskos, 2011). The only justification is that the severity of a response depends on its repulsion. But how is “repulsion” measured? Is repulsion analogous with deterrence – “shying away” from action for fear of punishment? If so, is it true in fact that self-help repulses more offenders than negotiation or avoidance? Is it, for example, worse to be slapped in the face (retaliation) or divorced (avoidance) than agree to pay a $1,000 fine (negotiation/compensation)? Cooney (2009: 204, fn7) himself writes that informal control “does not typically have a system of well-defined stages.”

It is possible that Cooney made a conceptual misstep in rank-ordering the forms of informal control as he did. This is especially important to us since our paper tests his theory of this conceptual ordering. Our analysis resulted in findings that are opposite of what his theory predicts. Perhaps the reason for this contradictory finding is that the four forms of informal control (retaliation, negotiation, avoidance, and toleration) cannot neatly be “arranged in order of how strongly they repel social actors” (Cooney, 2009: 10). Generally, there is contention on whether SP typologies are useful, harmful, or needless. Such classifications are counterproductive if they significantly “obscure differences in the effects of specific victim actions” (Tark & Kleck, 2002: 867). For example, distinct kinds of resistance have been found to have contrasting impacts on victims’ injury (Kleck & Delone, 1993). Other scholars argue that the benefit of typologies resides in their ability to simplify varieties of empirical information (Cooney and Phillips, 2002) and, in turn, allow for greater theoretical parsimony (see Popper, 2002 [1959]; Kuhn, 1977). In our mind, a middle-road to these two approaches is the best path: “less is more” given that “less is valid.” Future work should continue searching for that middle-road. Doing so involves empirically demonstrating or refuting the validity of Cooney’s rank ordering and also developing alternatives to his idea. 

References

Anderson, E. (1999). Code of the Street: Decency, Violence, and the Moral Life of the Inner City. New York: W.W. Norton & Company.

Authors. XXXX.

Bachman, R., Saltzman, L. E., Thompson, M. P., & Carmody, D. C. (2002). Disentangling the effects of self-protective behaviors on the risk of injury in assaults against women. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 18, 135-157.

Black, D. (1976). The Behavior of Law. New York: Academic Press.

Black, D. (1983). Crime as Social Control. American Sociological Review, 48, 34-45.

Black, D. (1995). The Epistemology of Pure Sociology. Law and Social Inquiry, 20, 829-870.

Black, D. (1998). The Social Structure of Right and Wrong, revised edition, San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Black, D. (2000). Dreams of Pure Sociology. Sociological Theory, 18, 343-367.

Black, D. (2011).  Moral Time. New York: Oxford University Press.

Block, R., and Skogan, W. G. (1986). Resistance and nonfatal outcomes in stranger-to-stranger predatory crime. Violence and Victims, 1, 241-253.

Clarke, R. V., & Cornish, D. B. (1985). Modeling Offenders’ Decisions: A Framework for Research and Policy. Crime and Justice, 6, 147-185.

Cooney, M. 1998. Warriors and Peacemakers: How Third Parties Shape Violence. New York: New York University Press.

Cooney, M. (2006). The Criminological Potential of Pure Sociology. Crime, Law, and Social Change, 46, 51-63.

Cooney, M. (2009). Is Killing Wrong? A Study in Pure Sociology. Charlottesville, VA: University of Virginia Press.

Cooney, M., & Phillips, S. (2002). Typologizing Violence: A Blackian Perspective. International Journal of Sociology and Social Policy, 22, 75-108.

Hirschi, Travis. 1969. Causes of Delinquency. Berkeley: University of California Press.

Hosmer, D. W., & Lemeshow, S. (2000). Applied Logistic Regression. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Jacobs, B. A., & Wright, R. (2006). Street Justice: Retaliation in the Criminal Underworld. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Jacques, S., & Wright, R. (2008). Intimacy with Outlaws: The Role of Relational Distance in Recruiting, Paying, and Interviewing Underworld Research Participants. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 45, 22-38.

Kleck, G., & DeLone, M. A. (1993). Victim Resistance and Offender Weapon Effects in Robbery. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 9, 55-81.

Kuhn, Thomas. (1977). The Essential Tension: Selected Studies in Scientific Tradition and Change. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Long, J. S. (1997). Regression Models for Categorical and Limited Dependent Variables. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Long, J. S. & Freese, J. (2005). Regression Models for Categorical Outcomes Using Stata, 2nd ed. College Station, TX: Stata Press.

Melde, C., & Rennison, C. M. (2010). Intimidation and Street Gangs: Understanding the Response of Victims and Bystanders to Perceived Gang Violence. Justice Quarterly, 27, 619-666.

Messner, S. F., & Rosenfeld, R. (2007). Crime and the American Dream, 4th ed. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.

Michalski, J. H. (2003). Financial Altruism or Unilateral Resource Exchanges? Toward a Pure Sociology of Welfare. Sociological Theory, 21, 341-358.

Moskos, P. (2011). In Defense of Flogging. New York: Basic Books.

Popper, K. (2002 [1959] ). The Logic of Scientific Discovery. New York: Routledge Classics.

Rennison, C. M., & Rand, M. (2007). Introduction to the National Crime Victimization Survey. In J. P. Lynch, & L. A. Addington (Eds.), Understanding Crime Statistics: Revisiting the Divergence of the NCVS and the UCR (17-54). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Straus, M. A. (1999). The Controversy Over Domestic Violence by Women. In X. B. Arriaga, & S. Oskamp. Violence in Intimate Relationships. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Tark, J., & Kleck, G. (2004). Resisting Crime: The Effects of Victim Action on the Outcomes of Crimes. Criminology, 42, 861-909.

Notes

[1] A fifth and sixth form of informal control are settlement and praise. The former is social control in which a neutral third-party intervenes (Black, 1998). Settlement may also be formal; i.e. involving the government. Praise is expressed social approval for a deviant act. Settlement is left out of the discussion and analysis for three reasons. One, this paper’s focus is on informal and reflexive responses by victims, but settlement behavior involves persons other than disputants; it may not occur during the victimization (i.e., be delayed); and, may involve the government (i.e., be formal). Second, the NCVS – which provides the data for analysis – does not allow for distinguishing between victim’s requests for informal versus formal settlement. Although participating victims are asked whether they “called police or guard”, they are not asked exactly which one they did; thus whether there response was formal or informal is not known. Similarly, praise is not considered in this paper because the NCVS does not explicitly collect information on whether the victim expressed approval for the crime against them, although this seems highly unlikely anyways. 

[2] It has also been used to study behaviors such as predation (Cooney, 2006; Cooney & Phillips, 2002), ideas (Black, 2000), welfare (Michalski 2003), art (Black, 1998), and research (Jacques & Wright, 2008). 

[3] This assertion does not deny that there are other factors that may play a part in SP, including but not limited to past history of victimization, gender, age, culture, racism and discrimination more broadly. 

[4] Cooney (2009) offers a sixth proposition on settlement as well.     

[5] For brevity’s sake we cannot provide more empirical examples of the theory we test. However, a plethora of them is provided in Cooney (2009) and thus we advise interested readers to refer to that work. 

[6] A possible conceptualization of relational distance is a three-category variable: intimate, other known, and stranger. This conceptualization indicates that ‘intimates’ are closer relationally than are ‘other knowns.’ Given the way NCVS data are collected, it is not possible to ensure this. Research routinely defines intimates as “current or former spouse, boyfriend or girlfriend”. The problem is that it is not clear that an ‘ex’ is closer relationally than some “other known” (e.g., one’s best friend) or even “stranger.” While it is possible to parse current and former spouses in the NCVS, this cannot be done with current and former boy- and girlfriends. Given this, we opted to go with the two-category presentation. This portrayal is accurately represents the theory under consideration and does not obscure any important findings. 

[7] All controls used in the models are described in Table I. Space limitations prohibit detailed descriptions of the measurement of control variables used in the analyses. These details are available from the authors upon request.

[8] We first considered using a proportional odds logistic regression model (Long, 1997; Hosmer & Lemeshow, 2000) given the conceptualization of reflexive informal control on an ordered continuum from low to high. We rejected this approach because it is not possible to test for the assumption of parallel regression using post-estimation techniques when using survey weighted procedures required by NCVS data. We did conduct post-estimation tests using non-survey weighted ordered logistic regression and findings indicated that the assumption of parallel regression was violated. When the assumption of parallel regression is violated, the selection of an alternative model without the restriction of parallel regressions is advised (Long, 1997; Long & Freese, 2005).

Tables

Comments
0
comment
No comments here
Why not start the discussion?